Sank's Glossary of Linguistics 
L-Lew

LABELING
(Syntax) Among the fundamental questions in minimalist research is why human language has φ-feature agreement and Case. Chomsky (2013) proposes a partial answer for this with his labeling algorithm. The operation Merge, which combines two elements α and β into {α, β}, is minimally required for language. This operation, he argues, must accompany an algorithm that specifies the nature of the formed object. For example, when a verbal element and a nominal element form a constituent, information must be provided whether the constituent is verbal (VP) or nominal (NP). His proposal is that φ-feature agreement plays a crucial role in this labeling process. On the other hand, it is proposed in Chomsky (2008), for example, that Case is necessary for φ-feature agreement and is valued through it. This leads to the picture in (1), where '→' means 'requires':

  1. Merge → Labeling → φ-feature agreement → Case
 These hypotheses successfully place φ-feature agreement and Case in the model of syntax in a way that is consistent with the strong minimalist thesis. At the same time, they present interesting research questions when languages like Japanese that are rich in Case but apparently lack φ-feature agreement are taken into consideration. | Mamoru Saito, 2016

LABELING ALGORITHM
(Syntax) Merge is defined as producing a simple set (i.e. Merge (α, β) = {α, β}), which we may call a syntactic object (SO). The rise of Merge over X'-schemata recaptures the aspect of discrete infinity in phrase structure as recursive application of Merge (i.e. Merge (γ, {α, β}) = {γ, {α, β}}), a property specific to human beings (cf. Fujita 2009). Merge does not entail the application of projection; rather, it purely ensures set formation, hence the labeling algorithm (LA) (Chomsky 2013):

The labeling algorithm (LA)
Suppose SO = {H, XP}, H a head and XP not a head. Then LA will select H as the label, and the usual procedures of interpretation at the interfaces can proceed.
 Since Merge yields an SO as a set but does not name it for interpretation at the interfaces, it follows that (1) emerges as an independent computational algorithm. LA detects an SO's internal head under minimal search and selects the detected head as the label of the SO. | Akihiko Sakamoto, 2013

LABOV'S ATTENTION-TO-SPEECH MODEL
(Sociolinguistics) Labov's "attention to speech" model (1972) holds that speakers shift styles in reaction to the formality of the speech situation.
   Stylistic variation is conditioned by how much attention speakers pay to their own speech as they converse. Speech registers, under this model, fall along a continuum according to self-consciousness of speech; less self-conscious varieties are labeled "casual" or "informal," and registers characterized by more self-consciousness are termed "careful" or "formal." Less self-conscious registers are also held to be further removed from standard or prestige language varieties than more self-conscious speech, which tends toward what the speaker perceives to be more standard speech. | Schilling-Estes, 1998

LABOV'S VERNACULAR PRINCIPLE
(Sociolinguistics) Labov's Vernacular Principle holds that the style which is most regular in its structure and its relation to the evolution of the language is the vernacular, in which the minimum attention is paid to speech. (1972) The Vernacular Principle has led sociolinguists to focus on speech which they determine to be non-self-conscious, at the expense of stylistic varieties such as performance speech, which are identified as self-conscious. | Schilling-Estes, 1998

LAMBDA (λ)

  1. (Semantics) A notion developed in mathematical logic and used as part of the conceptual apparatus underlying formal semantics. The lambda operator is a device which constructs expressions denoting functions out of other expressions (e.g. those denoting truth values) in a process called lambda abstraction. The process of relating equivalent lambda expressions is known as lambda conversion. Several kinds of lambda calculus have been devised as part of a general theory of functions and logic, functions here being defined as sets of unordered pairs (graphs). The approach has proved attractive to linguists because of its ability to offer a powerful system for formalizing exact meanings and semantic relationships, and lambda notions have helped to inform a number of linguistic theories, notably Montague grammar and categorial grammar. | David Crystal, 2008
  2. (Acoustics) The symbol for wavelength.

LAMBDA CALCULUS
(Mathematical Logic, Semantics) A formal system for expressing computation based on function abstraction and application using variable binding and substitution. It is a universal model of computation that can be used to simulate any Turing machine. It was introduced by the mathematician Alonzo Church in the 1930s as part of his research into the foundations of mathematics.
 Lambda calculus consists of constructing lambda terms and performing reduction operations on them. In the simplest form of lambda calculus, terms are built using only the following rules:

SyntaxNameDescription
xVariableA character or string representing
a parameter or mathematical/logical value.
(λx.M)AbstractionFunction definition (M is a lambda term).
The variable x becomes bound in the expression.
(M N)ApplicationApplying a function to an argument.
M and N are lambda terms.
producing expressions such as: (λx.λy.(λz.(λx.z x) (λy.z y)) (x y)). Parentheses can be dropped if the expression is unambiguous. For some applications, terms for logical and mathematical constants and operations may be included.
  The reduction operations include:
OperationNameDescription
(λx.M[x]) → (λy.M[y])α-conversionRenaming the bound variables
in the expression.
Used to avoid name collisions.
((λx.M) E) → (M[x := E])β-reductionReplacing the bound variables
with the argument expression
in the body of the abstraction.
 | Wikipedia, 2022

LAMINAL
(Phonetics) An articulation involving the blade of the tongue (= lamina). | Utrecht Lexicon of Linguistics, 2001

LANGUAGE CONVERGENCE
See CONVERGENCE, LANGUAGE.

LANGUAGE SHIFT
(Diachronic) Or, language transfer, or, language replacement, or, language assimilation. The process whereby a speech community shifts to a different language, usually over an extended period of time. Often, languages that are perceived to be higher status stabilize or spread at the expense of other languages that are perceived by their own speakers to be lower-status. An example is the shift from Gaulish to Latin during the time of the Roman Empire. | Wikipedia, 2022

LARYNGEAL FEATURES
(Phonetics) One proposal:

 | Scott R. Moisik and John H. Esling, 2011

LARYNGEAL NEUTRALIZATION
(Phonology) The most common phonological process involving laryngeal features is laryngeal neutralization, wherein all laryngeal distinctions are lost in syllable-final position. | Linda Lombardi, 1995

LATE-PEAK ACCENT

  1. (Prosody) In early-peak or late-peak accents, f0 peaks either precede or follow the stressed syllable they are associated with. | Katharina Zahner, Sophie Kutscheid, and Bettina Braun, 2019
  2. (Prosody) Previous research has demonstrated that nuclear intonation contours in German crucially differ with respect to f0-peak alignment (Kohler 1991, Grice et al. 2005, Niebuhr 2022). The f0 peak may either precede the stressed syllable (H+L*, early-peak accent), or follow it (L*+H, late-peak accent), or be aligned within the stressed syllable (L+H*, medial-peak accent). | Katharina Zahner-Ritter, Marieke Einfeldt, Daniela Wochner, Angela James, Nicole Dehé, and Bettina Braun, 2022

LEFT BRANCH CONDITION
(Syntax) Ross (1967, 1986) proposed the Left Branch Condition (LBC), which blocks movement of the leftmost constituent of an NP. The condition has been used in the literature to block extraction of determiners, possessors, and adjectives out of NP.

  1. * Whosei did you see [ ti father ]?
  2. * Whichi did you buy [ ti car ]?
  3. * Thati he saw [ ti car ].
  4. * Beautifuli he saw [ ti houses ].
  5. * How muchi did she earn [ ti money ]?
 | Željko Bošković, 2005

LEFT-BRANCH EXTRACTION

  1. (Syntax) When an element (located in the left branch of a larger NP or DP) is moved via A-bar movement. | Carol-Rose Little, 2020
  2. (Syntax) As noted by Ross, some languages, e.g., Latin and most Slavic languages (Ross 1986 notes this for Russian), allow LBE, as illustrated by Serbo-Croatian and Latin. (Pied-piping of the LBE remnant is also possible. (6) was provided by an anonymous reviewer and (7) is taken from Uriagereka 1988.)
    1. Serbo-Croatian
      Čijegi
      whose
      si
      are
      vidio
      seen
      [ ti
       
      oca ]?
      father
      'Whose father did you see?'
    2. Kakvai
      what-kind-of
      si
      are
      kupio
      bought
      [ ti
       
      kola ]?
      car
      'What kind of a car did you buy?'
    3. Tai
      that
      je
      is
      vidio
      seen
      [ ti
       
      kola ].
      car
      'That car, he saw.'
    4. Lijepei
      beautiful
      je
      is
      vidio
      seen
      [ ti
       
      kuće ].
      houses
      'Beautiful houses, he saw.'
    5. Kolikoi
      how-much
      je
      is
      zaradila
      earned
      [ ti
       
      novca ]?
      money
      'How much money did she earn?'
    6. Latin
      Cuiami
      whose
      amat
      loves
      Cicero
      Cicero
      [ ti
       
      puellam ]?
      girl
      'Whose girl does Cicero love?'
    7. Qualesi
      what-kind-of
      Cicero
      Cicero
      amat
      loves
      [ ti
       
      puellas ]?
      girls
      'What kind of girls does Cicero love?'
     | Željko Bošković, 2005

LEFT DISLOCATION

  1. (Syntax) The construction of e.g. (1):
    1. This next man, have I got to see him?
     Distinguished from simple fronting (2) by a pronoun (him) or other anaphoric element in the normal position of the dislocated element.
    1. This next man have I got to see?
     | Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics, 2007
  2. (Syntax) The sentence My mother, she is a good person is an example of a construction where a non-vocative noun phrase in initial position is set off from a following sentence that contains one or more pronouns coreferential with the initial NP. Since Haj Ross's 1967 dissertation Constraints on Variables in Syntax, this construction has been known as left dislocation. Haj's examples included:
    1. The man my father works with in Boston, he's going to tell the police that the traffic expert has set that traffic light on the corner of Murk Street far too low.
    2. My father, he's Armenian, and my mother, she's Greek.
    3. My wife, somebody stole her handbag last night.
    When the pronoun is sentence-initial, you might think that it's in apposition with the left-dislocated noun phrase. But this generally makes neither semantic nor prosodic sense. If the structure were really
    My father – he – 's Armenian.
    the phrasing would be different (and strange), and it's hard to see why one would want to add the pronoun, which would add none of the parallel information that appositives usually do.
     The third example (My wife, somebody stole her handbag last night) illustrates the fact that the pronoun need not be adjacent to the left-dislocated noun at all. (Though examples with the pronoun in subject position are by far the commonest.)
     Constructions of this general type are common across the languages of the world, and in so-called topic-prominent languages, they're the norm. The left-dislocation structure is often said to divide the sentence into topic and comment, or some similar sort of articulation of information. (This works when the initial item is a full noun phrase, referenced in the following sentence by a pronoun or pronouns, but it wouldn't work the other way around. There's a lot more to be said about the pragmatics of LD, and there's a large literature discussing it. | Mark Liberman, 2008

LEFT PART STRANDING
(Grammar) A novel ellipsis phenomenon that deletes the second part of compounds, stranding the left side of the compound in sentence-final position:

  1. Deze
    this
    lift
    lift
    is
    is
    zevenpersoons,
    seven.person.ADJ
    en
    and
    die
    that
    acht
    eight
    ___ (Dutch)
    'This lift can carry seven people, and that one can carry eight people.'
 We refer to this phenomenon as left part stranding (LPS for short) and show that it occurs both in Dutch and Hungarian in very similar ways and can affect the second part of an adjectival compound with a derivational affix. In addition to the fact that LPS curiously violates Lexical Integrity and cannot be classified as any known exception to this condition (such as coordination reduction, eliminating the first part of a compound in a second coordinand, or the second part of a compound in a first coordinand, see Booij 1985), LPS is also curious in that it has many unexpected properties, which are atypical of any process of coordination reduction:
  1. It is only possible under clausal but not phrasal coordination.
  2. It preferably occurs in compounds that are the adjectival predicate of a clause.
  3. It preferably co-occurs with gapping or TP-ellipsis (fragments, sluicing).
  4. The stranded part is necessarily contrastive.
 | Anikó Lipták and Crit Cremers, 2023

LEFT PERIPHERY
See PERIPHERY.

LEFT UPWARD/DOWNWARD MONOTONICITY
See MONOTONICITY.

LEMMA
(Lexicology) A glossed word or phrase. | Merriam-Webster

LENIS
See FORTIS.

LEVEL TONE
(Phonology) Or, register tone. Tone that doesn't change pitch. | Zita McRobbie-Utasi, 2011

LEVELING
(Sociolinguistics) The eradication of marked or minority forms in situations of dialect competition, where the number of variants in the output is dramatically reduced from the number in the input. | David Britain, 2001

LEVELING, MORPHOLOGICAL
(Morphology) Or, paradigm leveling. The generalization of an inflection across a linguistic paradigm, a group of forms with the same stem in which each form corresponds in usage to different syntactic environments (SIL 2015), or between words (Singh 2005). The result of such leveling is a paradigm that is less varied, having fewer forms (Hazen, 2014).
 When a language becomes less synthetic, it is often a matter of morphological leveling. An example is the conjugation of English verbs, which has become almost unchanging today, thus contrasting sharply, for example, with Latin, in which one verb has dozens of forms, each one expressing a different tense, aspect, mood, voice, person, and number. For instance, English sing has only two forms in the present tense (I/you/we/they sing and he/she sings), but its Latin equivalent cantāre has six: one for each combination of person and number. | Wikipedia, 2022

 

Page Last Modified January 30, 2024

 
B a c k   T o   I n d e x